Wednesday, August 21, 2013

Home-Invasion Robbery: Contemplating the Legal and Statute Disparities and Neurological Implications Regarding Criminality






Home Invasion Robberies
By: Dr. Peter A. Barone

Home-invasion robbery involves an entry made into a person’s home and in the states that do not have a home-invasion robbery statute, the charge applied to the individual categorically falls under a burglary, residential robbery, or an aggravated robbery statute. However, upon examining the crime it is apparent that a home-invasion robbery differs from burglary or a residential robbery in that its perpetrators have a violent intent apart from the unlawful entry itself, specific or general, much the same way as aggravated robbery which is personally taking from someone by force, a charge differentiated from mere larceny, which would be considered to be theft alone.  Few statistics are available on the crime of home-invasion as such, due to the fact that it is not defined as a crime in its own right in most jurisdictions.  Statistics about home-invasion robbery found on the internet are often false or misleading alike.  Many times, these types of crimes are captured in the category of burglary which is an inaccurate depiction of the criminal actions being perpetrated by these offenders.  This inaccurate reporting contributes to the inability of accurately obtaining the actual number of home-invasion robberies that have taken place and allows for the escape of critical demographic and operational information necessary for assessment and analysis of this criminal activity.
Home Invasion Robbery in the State of Florida
In 2003, the state legislators in Florida saw that the crime of invading the home of a person, then confronting them in their own home and subsequently robbing them as being so egregious that these actions warranted concentration towards such an accentuated crime and that this type of crime carried a greater penalty for its’ commission than just a burglary (Florida House of Representatives HB 483 Session, 2003). Not all states have followed suit notwithstanding the insulting and violating nature of this criminal act and the total disregard for the sanctity of one’s home and terror visited upon the victims of being threatened, attacked and robbed within the sanctuary of their own homes. 
 Home-Invasion Robbery in the State of Florida, F.S. 812.135. In the State of Florida, if a person is charged with a burglary, they are facing a second degree felony which is punishable by a maximum penalty of 15 years in state prison. If a person is charged with a strong armed robbery, without a weapon, they are facing a second degree felony which is punishable by a maximum penalty of 15 years in state prison. In the State of Florida, if a person commits a home-invasion robbery they are charged with a First Degree Felony and a level 8 which results in points on their score sheet and is greater than the burglary or robbery if committed as separate crimes. In the State of Florida if the person commits a home-invasion robbery and is either armed or arms themselves during the robbery they can be sentenced to life in prison (F.S. 812.135).
Home-Invasion Robbery in the State of Illinois. In the State of Illinois, the legislature also decided that the act of burglarizing and then robbing the individuals who were home at the time should be considered to be more egregious an act than robbing someone on the street where a person does not have that same sense and feeling of safety and security provided by being within the four walls of their residence. Illinois also established that a home -invasion was more of a traumatic event than a burglary as an offender proceeds to enter an unoccupied residence to pilferage a person’s belongings without the propensity for a person to person confrontation (720 ILCS 5/12-11) (from Ch. 38, par. 12-11).
Illinois’s Home-Invasion Robbery Statute 720 ILCS 5/12-11. In the State of Illinois, the legislature has also created a home-invasion statute that provides for more serious penalties for an individual who commits and is convicted of perpetrating a home-invasion robbery. In fact, similar to the State of Florida, the State of Illinois has also provided for more enhanced penalties for the commission of violent acts, being armed, or arming yourself during the commission of a home-invasion robbery (720 ILCS 5/12-11).
In the State of Illinois, if a person is charged with and convicted with the crime of home-invasion robbery they can be sentenced to state prison for a term of 30 – 60 years. If it is proven that during the commission of the home-invasion robbery the person was armed, with a firearm, and used the firearm or they threaten to use force on any of the occupants of the residence, the perpetrator is subject to an additional 15 years that can be added onto the sentence by the sentencing judge. If the perpetrator used or threatened the imminent use of force to anyone in the residence instead of the 15 years being added onto the 30 – 60 years another 20 years can be added onto the sentence. (730 ILCS 5/5-8-1(a)(3)). The State of Illinois goes even further and states that if the perpetrators during the commission of an home-invasion robbery are armed with a firearm and during the home-invasion robbery, personally discharges a firearm that proximately causes great bodily harm, permanent disability or disfigurement, or death to another person in the dwelling the person will have 25 years to life added onto the original 30-60 years in state prison (McCarthy, 2008).
In fact, the State of Illinois deems a home-invasion robbery to be such a serious offense that it has classified it as a Class X Felony along with such crimes as Aggravated Kidnapping, Aggravated Criminal Sexual Assault, and Aggravated Vehicular Hijacking. In the State of Illinois, a  Class X felony is, short of first degree murder, the most serious felony offense on the books in Illinois. Upon a finding of guilt, the court cannot sentence the defendant to probation. The offense has a mandatory minimum sentence of 6-30 years in the Department of Corrections. Even a first-time offender who has no criminal background is subject to a mandatory prison sentence. It is not a matter of the judge’s discretion in sentencing (730 ILCS 5/5-8-1(a)(3)).  In addition to the aforementioned, in the State of Illinois a person charged and convicted of a class X felony does not have  probation as an option under Illinois law for a Class X felony offense (730 ILCS 5/5-5-3(c)(2)(C)).
Applicable Theories of Crime
People learn behavior via observation, acting out the behavior, and then being rewarded by successfully accomplishing the reenactment of the behavior (Bandura, 1986, 1997, 2001). When learned behavior is positively reinforced, whether it is socially acceptable or unacceptable, it is something that will continue until the individual feels it is no longer a benefit to them or it stops being reinforced by those around them who value their recognition. The more value a person places on the approval of their actions the more solidified the learning becomes and the stronger the desire and need becomes to perform that learned behavior. Learning theories tend to follow the lead of Edwin Sutherland's theory of differential association, developed in 1947, although ideas about imitation or modeling go back to 1890.  Often oversimplified as "peer group" theories, learning is much more than that, and involves the analysis of what is positively and negatively rewarding (reinforcing) for individuals (Williams & McShane, 1998).
Differential Association Theory
When examining Edwin H. Sutherland's theory of Differential Association it becomes   evident that it can be categorized as a learning theory. The basic premise of this theory is that criminal behavior is acquired through the learning process, just as is lawful behavior. The theory demonstrates that the socialization process is essentially the same, regardless of whether the messages being transmitted are conventional or deviant. It is through the interaction with others that people learn attitudes which are both favorable and unfavorable as relates to the violation of laws. Sutherland claims that a person turns to criminal behavior when there is an excess of attitudes and values favoring law violation as opposed to adherence to the law (Sutherland, 1947). 
In theory, and as a learning theory, Differential Association is one of the most logical explanations of criminal behavior. When using differential association to explain criminal behavior the researcher must look at criminal behavior after it has taken place with the goal of reconstructing the reality of the criminal's world (Lilly, Cullen & Ball, 1995). The actions demonstrated by the majority of the individuals involved in home-invasion robberies from both cities are directly in line with what Sutherland’s belief that criminal behavior developed from associating with those individuals who commit crimes and that commission of crimes  was more likely to result from learned behaviors. Motivations and technical knowledge, according to Sutherland, were likely to be learned from those who commit crimes.


The principle of Differential Association asserts that a person becomes delinquent because they are constantly exposed to a surplus of definitions favorable to violation(s) of law over definitions unfavorable to the violation(s) of law. In other words, criminal behavior emerges when a person is exposed to more social messages from those around them favoring conduct that is anti-social rather than pro-social (Sutherland, 1947). According to Tittle (1986, p. 429), "despite some important anomalies, our findings support the major theme of Sutherland's thinking. Association with criminal definitions does seem to be a generator of crime, and it appears to exercise its influence indirectly through its effects on a learned symbolic construct-motivation to engage in criminal behavior."
The learning process posited in Differential Association involves the same mechanisms whether a person is learning criminal behavior or learning conformity to social norms and laws. The data collected from both law enforcement agencies demonstrate that the majority of the home-invasion robberies were committed by more than two perpetrators. These groups can provide the social messages from those around them favoring conduct that is anti-social rather than pro-social as discussed by Sutherland (1947) in his explanation of the foundational tenet in Differential Association.
Perpetrators of home-invasion robberies commit these crimes and there exists a very good possibility that they are aware that these are considered to be very serious offenses with very serious consequences attached to them if they are caught, prosecuted and convicted. However, the value they place on committing these acts with their peers outweighs the rational thought process or decision making process that someone about to engage in any activity with a gain and loss ratio would see as very costly and reconsider engaging in, once the situation was evaluated. The more often these activities are performed, the more times a benefit is gained, and the more acceptance by the group and peers, the more reinforced the learned behavior is solidified as being acceptable notwithstanding the critically devastating consequences. This was supported in as far back as 1968 in a study by Matthews.


In 1968, Victor Matthews conducted a study among a group of high school students (boys) in the mid-west. He utilized two instruments, a personal data sheet and a delinquency scale similar to the one used by Short. The emphasis was on the degree of delinquent involvement by an individual as compared to that of his peers. Matthews found that uniformity in behavior was greatest between best friends. He concluded that the greater the degree of identification between individuals, the greater will be the degree of uniformity of their self-reported behaviors.  Again, the theory of Differential Association was supported (Mathews, 1968). The data from the Chicago and Miami-Dade Police Departments home-invasion robbery reports appear to support the gender and age findings provided by the theory. 
Social Learning Theory
Social learning theory (Bandura, 1963, 2011), Rotter (1982, 1990), and Differential Association created by Sutherland (1947) were created and geared toward the explanation of the commission of crimes by youth which supports what is posited by Blumstein, Cohen, Roth, and Visher (1986) and Baker (1998).  These theorists advised that males are more likely to commit crime than females and this is especially true when discussing serious crimes. These authors also advised that the commission of crimes is pre-eminently a preoccupation of the young, whether male or female, which is also borne out by the findings of the two studies conducted for this research from both law enforcement agencies.
Hirschi (1969) advised that crime is overwhelmingly committed by people in the age range 15-24 and with this information we would expect crime to increase with the number of people in this population age range. Blumstein, Cohen, Roth, and Visher (1986) and Baker (1998) also advised that financial gain is a strong driving force behind the commission of the home-invasion robberies. The studies show that the perpetration of home-invasion robberies, are committed by males in groups on Friday nights between midnight and 5:00 AM with the motive being financial and the age of the males being 20 to 33 years.
In addition to all of the aforementioned information being borne out by data from both police agencies, there is another issue that needs to be discussed which relates to the possible understanding of why the individuals who perpetrate these home-invasion robberies still perform these actions with the existence of such severe penalties for the crime of home-invasion and for the lesser included crimes. An issue that is relevant in the attempt to understand the actions of the perpetrators of the home-invasion robberies examined in this paper involves the brain and its physiology. 
Lack of Deterrence in the Face of Severe Punishment
This action directly in contradiction to the existing consequences are vividly addressed in the writings of Fletcher (1978) where he echoes the extremely well-recognized and accepted ideas of the diverse rationale regarding punishment falling into two valid classifications pertaining to the justification of criminal sanctions.  In the predicted consequences of condemning the particular defendant as a criminal and depriving him or her of their liberty, the rationale of both general and specific deterrence is applicable. He further addresses the concepts of general deterrence being the punishment visited upon a specific defendant serving as a deterrence of those observing to be influenced to not commit such crimes and specific deterrence which has its’ value in the desire to prevent the specific individual being charged, convicted and sentenced to not be of the mind to commit these types of crimes in the future. In his writings, Fletcher (1978 p. 414) astutely posits that “All of these predicted goods are highly speculative.” As he is referring to the issue of attempting to predict what amount of punishment will be sufficient to deter a person that is highly determined on committing such an egregious act against a fellow member of society.  
The results of the assessment of the data collected in these studies supports the discussion and conclusions of Fletcher (1978) and presents a need for the assessment and classification of these perpetrators into typologies of offenders for a better understanding and more feasible method of dealing with these types of perpetrators by those commissioned to investigate, arrest, charge, try, and sentence these perpetrators of home-invasion robberies. There is also a need for a theoretical explanation of such contraindicated behavior(s) when elements of sentencing for the additional actions of violence, threats, arming with a firearm and discharging of firearms during the commission of home-invasion robberies subsist. Any of these charges could also be used in a plea bargain between the state and the defense.
Lesser Included Charges for Home-Invasion Robberies
 Lesser Included Charges in the State of Florida
In the State of Florida, the crimes listed below would be applicable if the person had a weapon and if someone was threatened or actually struck with or without the weapon. The category one charges must be listed on the verdict form, and the category two offenses are discussed and then can be agreed upon by the prosecution and the defense. All of the charges for the lesser have to be approved by the trial judge (Beckstrum, 2012).
Lesser Included Offenses in Florida
Table 1
HOME-INVASION ROBBERY — 812.135
CATEGORY ONE
CATEGORY TWO
FLA. STAT.
INS. NO.
Robbery with a weapon

812.13(2)(b)

Robbery

812.13(2)(c)
15.1
Burglary

810.02(4)
13.1

Aggravated battery
784.045
8.4

Battery
784.03
8.3

Aggravated assault
784.021
8.2

Assault
784.011
8.1

Attempt
777.04(1)
5.1

Burglary
810.02(3)
13.1

Trespass
810.08
13.3
Petit theft

812.014(3)(a)
14.1

Petit theft
812.014(2)(e)
14.1

Lesser Included Charges in Illinois
In Illinois, courts have identified three possible methods for determining whether a certain offense is a lesser included offense of another: (1) the "charging instruments" approach; (2) the "abstract elements" approach; and (3) the "factual" or "evidence" adduced at trial approach (People v. Novak, 1994). In People v. Miller (2010), the Supreme Court of Illinois held that “the abstract elements approach governs whether a charged offense is a lesser included offense of another charged offense” (Miller, 2010 p.166). Under the abstract elements approach, if all the elements of one offense are included in a second offense and the first offense contains no element not included in the second offense, the first offense is a lesser included offense of the second (Miller, 2010 p. 166). This test, while easy to apply, is more rigid than the charging instrument approach and it basically advises that it must be impossible to commit the greater offense without necessarily committing the lesser offense (People v. Novak, 2010 at 106; People v. Nunez 2010). In Illinois, when a defendant perpetrates a home-invasion robbery the lesser included crimes would be burglary, robbery or petit theft which are similar to the lesser included crimes in the State of Florida.  The lesser included crimes for home-invasion robbery in both states are serious offenses and carry serious sentences, except for the petit theft charges, and notwithstanding the sentences for these offenses the crimes are still committed. 
The Neurological Perspective of Home Invaders Behaviors
In studies conducted by Bjork, Smith, Danube, and Hommer (2007) and Nelson, McClure, Monk, Leibenluft, and Pine (2000), proved that by employing brain imaging technology, adolescents have a heightened propensity for risk-taking and poor decision-making correlates with immature cortical brain function, juvenile offending is less blameworthy by virtue of having some type of a biological basis to it. In addition, it is also asserted upon that young teens are neurologically and emotionally hard-wired for seeking out sensation-seeking, impulsive activity. In addition, juveniles and young adults can also demonstrate very poor foresight, bad judgment, and a failure to control their action(s) which can result in a skewed, non-realistic, self-serving and instantaneous gratification, decision-making thought process. This could result in the dismissal of the severe sentencing sanctions which are connected to the conviction for perpetrating a home-invasion robbery and the use of a firearm or implementation of force on the victims during the perpetration of this crime.


The Brain and Physiology
Differences in cognition, behavior, and emotions between children, adolescents, and adults have been noted as being in existence for millennia. The temporal lobes, amygdala, and hippocampus sub-serve emotion, language, and memory, and these are in control of functions that change markedly between the ages of 4 and 18 years (Jerslid, 1963; Wechsler, 1974; Diener et al., 1985). Scientific research has demonstrated that the full development of the brain does not manifest until the age of 25.
When aligning this information with the data collected from both law enforcement agencies used for this research it is feasible that the aforementioned factors can assist in the explaining of the actions of the perpetrators of home-invasion robberies from both states. This is especially true in relation to the age and number of defendants, the possession and use of weapons to include firearms, and the aggressiveness demonstrated by the perpetrators of these crimes. All of these physiological factors appear to be in conformity with and would assist in explaining the reasons why the individuals perpetrating these crimes would take the chances they do, knowing the penalties they are facing if arrested and convicted.
In addition, the violent acts of kicking in the front door, possessing a firearm and then discharging that firearm, at and upon the victims of home-invasion robberies, when facing these same stiff penalties would be additionally explained via a physiological approach and perspective. In addition to physiological aspects of the brain there is the issue involving responses by individuals involving pattern recognition. Gobet and Simon (1996) concluded that chess skill, for world-class players at least, does not deteriorate much when thinking time is substantially reduced. Advocates of this fairly extreme view have proposed that “recognition, by allowing knowledge to be accessed rapidly, allows the slower look-ahead search to be greatly abridged or even dispensed with entirely without much loss in quality of play” (Gobet & Simon, 1996 p. 53).

Pattern Recognition
Traditionally, scientists have treated patternicity as an error in a person’s cognition (Shermer, 2008). Scientists describe different types of errors such as a type I error, or a false positive which is described as believing something is real when it is not, which is like finding a nonexistent pattern. Scientists describe a type II error, or what is called a false negative, as actually not believing something is real when it is recognizing a real pattern; this is referred to as a phenomenon entitled “apatternicity”. In Shermer’s book How We Believe (Times Books, 2000), he posits that our brains are actual belief engines and they evolve to be pattern-recognition machines that connect the dots and create meaning out of the patterns that we think we see in nature. Sometimes when we see A, the letter A is really connected to the letter B; however, sometimes this is not the actual case. When it is, the person may have learned something valuable regarding the environment and the experience from which they can make predictions that aid in both survival and reproduction. We are the ancestors of those people who were most successful at finding patterns with this process being called association learning. Scientists posit that this is also fundamental to all animal behavior which ranges from the little earth worm to Homosapiens.
It can be argued that what is happening here is called pattern recognition which is discussed by Bishop (1995). When examining the activities of individuals, it is understood that people are classifier systems that process and store patterns of concepts and experiences they encounter for future recognition of these patterns and things previously observed and processed (Rosenberg, 2005). The pattern recognition process is one of the distinguishing features of neural network (NN) systems that people use to classify patterns in life. This process is accomplished in an interesting fashion in that rather than being cued by theory or explanatory goals, NNs are cued by what are called stereotypical training sets. In effect, in order to see patterns, you need to have prior patterns to train your NN (Bishop. 1995). 
The issue here is that if the individual’s brain, or if the NN recognizes a pattern, or believes it recognizes it and applies that pattern to what the person is experiencing, then the brain can be convinced that certain actions or responses are warranted; however, it may not actually be the proper response to what is actually transpiring at the time. Furthermore, the pattern recognition process could also explain why the perpetrators of these home invasion robberies take the aggressive and violent actions notwithstanding the severe sanctions they are facing. Combining all of these factors and theories together can provide a feasible explanation for the criminal actions involved in home-invasion robberies. This does not however relieve the perpetrators of responsibilities; but it could provide an understanding of the perpetrators’ actions especially if all of the perpetrators who are acting together happen to have similar experiences in regards to their subjective neural networks.                                          
References

"A Review of State Home Invasion Laws in the U.S.". Home invasion news. (July, 2011). Retrieved from, http://www.homeinvasionnews.com/a-review-of-home-invasion-laws-in-the-u-s/.

Baker, J. 1998, Juveniles in crime - Part 1: Participation rates and risk factors, NSW Bureau         of Crime Statistics and Research, Sydney.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentive perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1-26.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: W.H. Freeman.Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:                Prentice-Hall.
Beckstrom, G., (2012). Florida Criminal Jury Instruction Handbook, 2012 – 2013 Edition. Lexis Nexis.

Bishop, Christopher M. 1995. Neural networks for pattern recognition. Oxford, New York:           Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press.

Blumstein, A., Cohen, J., Roth, J.A. & Visher, C.A. 1986, ‘Criminal Careers and
Career Criminals,’ vol. 1, National Academy Press, p. 40.

Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Larsen, R.F., 1985. Age and sex effects for affect intensity. Developmental Psychology 21, 542–546.

Fletcher, G. (1778). Rethinking criminal law. Boston, MA: Little Brown and Company.

Florida House of Representatives HB 483 Session. (2003). Retrieved from, http://www.myfloridahouse.gov/sections/Bills/billsdetail.aspx?BillId=10067

Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look-ahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from grand-master-level chess. Psychological Science, 7, 52–55.

Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency, University of California Press, California.

Hunter, N., & Marshall, J. (2000). Robbery in a dwelling or home invasion? Analysis of 1998 police incident reports. Office of Crime Statistics for South Australia. Retrieved from, http://www.ocsar.sa.gov.au/docs/information_bulletins/IB12.pdf.

Hutton, E. (2012). Investigating the criminality of home-invasion robbery: Establishing baseline offender typologies. Capella University.

Illinois Complied Statutes (720 ILCS 5/12-11) (from Ch. 38, par. 12-11) Retrieved from, http://law.onecle.com/illinois/720ilcs5/12-11.html.

Jernigan, T.L., Tallal, P. (1990). Late childhood changes in brain morphology observable with MRI. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 32, 379–385.

Lilly, J., Cullen, F., & .Ball, R (1995). Criminological theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Matthews, V. (1968). Differential identification: An empirical note, Social Problems, 1, 383.

Matsueda, R. L, (1988). The current state of differential association theory. Crime and Delinquency, Sage Publication

McCarthy, K.E. (2008).  Research report: Burglary; Felonies; Legislation; Sentencing; Persistent Offenders; Trespassing. Retrieved from, http://www.cga.ct.gov/2008/rpt/2008-R-0088.htm

Illinois Complied Statutes 730 ILCS 5/5-5-3(c)(2)(C) (2013).

Illinois Complied Statutes 730 ILCS 5/5-8-1(a)(3) (2013).

People v. Miller, 238 Ill. 2d 161 (2010),

People v. Nunez, 236 Ill. 2d 488 (2010).

People v. Novak, 163 Ill. 2d 93 (1994).

Rosenberg, A. (2005). Philosophy of science: A contemporary introduction. (2nd ed.), Routledge contemporary introductions to philosophy. New York; London: Routledge.

Rotter, J. B. (1990). Internal versus external control of reinforcement. American Psychologist, 45, 489-493.
Rotter, J. B. (1982). The development and applications of social learning theory. New York: Praeger.

Shermer, M. B., (2008). Patternicity: Finding meaningful patterns in meaningless noise why the brain believes something is real when it is not. Scientific American Magazine, November Issue, Retrieved from, http://jotamac.typepad.com/files/patternicity_-finding-meaningful-patterns-in-meaningless-noise_-scientific-american.pdf

Shermer, M. B., (2000). How we believe. The search for God in an age of science. New York, NY: Times Books.
Sutherland, E. H., (1947). Principles of criminology. 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: J.B. Lippincott.

Voss, H.L. (1968). Differential association and reported delinquent behavior: A replication, Social Problems, 964, p. 85, col. 2. 63 20
Williams, F. & McShane, M. (1998). Criminological theory. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.

Wechsler, D., 1974. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised.The Psychological Corporation, New York.